When laboratory studies involve music, it is often critical to
have input from musicians in order to create ecologically valid test conditions
and stimuli. For example, in a current study underway with Charles, we
are examining emotional expression in jazz improvisation. Input from musicians
(such as Mike Pope) was fundamental in determining our experimental design. Although
we studied jazz musicians and drew on their insights, the results ultimately show more about
creativity and emotional expression as general abilities than they do about
music or jazz more specifically. Our results don’t immediately provide
information of practical use to the musicians we studied, or to artists more
generally.
Intersections between art and neuroscience are not always equally
productive for both disciplines. Art can be a means for scientific inquiry, but
neuroscientists cannot assume that by incorporating art forms into our studies,
we are somehow helping or informing artists. Likewise, art inspired by
neuroscience is not inherently helpful or informative to neuroscientists
(though it certainly can be). We should not expect that every interaction
between art and neuroscience can (or should) be equally beneficial to both
fields.
While it is important to foster mutual constructive relationships between neuroscience and art, there is a significant place
for commensal exchanges, where one discipline may benefit more than the other.
A common theme throughout the conference was that artists and
neuroscientists often talk past one another. They have
different reference points, lexicons, assumptions, motivations and goals. In
any collaboration between the arts and neuroscience, it is important to engage
with and build out from these differences. The scope of collaborations should be assessed
critically, with an awareness of the distinct backgrounds and objectives
of artists and scientists, and the varying outcomes for each field.
No comments:
Post a Comment